>Childhood Syndrome > >-------------------------------------------------------------------- > >THE ETIOLOGY & TREATMENT OF CHILDHOOD > >Jordan W. Smoller, University of Pennsylvania > >Childhood is a syndrome which has only recently begun to receive serious >attention from clinicians. The syndrome itself, however, is not at all >recent. As early as the 8th century, the Persian historian Kidnom made >references to "short, noisy creatures," who may well have been what we now >call "children." The treatment of children, however, was unknown until this >century, when so-called "child psychologists" and "child psychiatrists" >became common. Despite this history of clinical neglect, it has been >estimated that well over half of all Americans alive today have experienced >childhood directly (Suess, 1983). In fact, the actual numbers are probably >much higher, since these data are based on self-reports which may be subject >to social desirability biases and retrospective distortion. > >The growing acceptance of childhood as a distinct phenomenon is reflected in >the proposed inclusion of the syndrome in the upcoming Diagnostic and >Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, or DSM-IV, of the >American Psychiatric Association (1990). Clinicians are still in disagreement >about the significant clinical features of childhood, but the proposed DSM-IV >will almost certainly include the following core features: > > * Congenital onset > * Dwarfism > * Emotional lability and immaturity > * Knowledge deficits > * Legume anorexia > >Clinical Features of Childhood: >Although the focus of this paper is on the efficacy of conventional treatment >of childhood, the five clinical markers mentioned above merit further >discussion for those unfamiliar with this patient population. > >CONGENITAL ONSET > >In one of the few existing literature reviews on childhood, Temple-Black >(1982) has noted that childhood is almost always present at birth, although >it may go undetected for years or even remain subclinical indefinitely. This >observation has led some investigators to speculate on a biological >contribution to childhood. As one psychologist has put it, "we may soon be in >a position to distinguish organic childhood from functional childhood" >(Rogers, 1979). > >DWARFISM > >This is certainly the most familiar marker of childhood. It is widely known >that children are physically short relative to the population at large. >Indeed, common clinical wisdom suggests that the treatment of the so-called >"small child" (or "tot") is particularly difficult. These children are known >to exhibit infantile behavior and display a startling lack of insight (Tom >and Jerry, 1967). > >EMOTIONAL LABILITY AND IMMATURITY > >This aspect of childhood is often the only basis for a clinician's diagnosis. >As a result, many otherwise normal adults are misdiagnosed as children and >must suffer the unnecessary social stigma of being labelled a "child" by >professionals and friends alike. > >KNOWLEDGE DEFICITS > >While many children have IQ's with or even above the norm, almost all will >manifest knowledge deficits. Anyone who has known a real child has >experienced the frustration of trying to discuss any topic that requires some >general knowledge. Children seem to have little knowledge about the world >they live in. Politics, art, and science -- children are largely ignorant of >these. Perhaps it is because of this ignorance, but the sad fact is that most >children have few friends who are not, themselves, children. > >LEGUME ANOREXIA > >This last identifying feature is perhaps the most unexpected. Folk wisdom is >supported by empirical observation -- children will rarely eat their >vegetables (see Popeye, 1957, for review). > >Causes of Childhood: > >Now that we know what it is, what can we say about the causes of childhood? >Recent years have seen a flurry of theory and speculation from a number of >perspectives. Some of the most prominent are reviewed below. > >Sociological Model > >Emile Durkheim was perhaps the first to speculate about sociological causes >of childhood. He points out two key observations about children: > 1) the vast majority of children are unemployed, and > 2) children represent one of the least educated segments of our society. > >In fact, it has been estimated that less than 20% of children have had more >than fourth grade education. > >Clearly, children are an "out-group." Because of their intellectual handicap, >children are even denied the right to vote. From the sociologist's >perspective, treatment should be aimed at helping assimilate children into >mainstream society. Unfortunately, some victims are so incapacitated by their >childhood that they are simply not competent to work. One promising >rehabilitation program (Spanky and Alfalfa, 1978) has trained victims of >severe childhood to sell lemonade. > >Biological Model > >The observation that childhood is usually present from birth has led some to >speculate on a biological contribution. An early investigation by Flintstone >and Jetson (1939) indicated that childhood runs in families. Their survey of >over 8,000 American families revealed that over half contained more than one >child. Further investigation revealed that even most non-child family members >had experienced childhood at some point. Cross-cultural studies (e.g., Mowgli >& Din, 1950) indicate that family childhood is even more prevalent in the Far >East. For example, in Indian and Chinese families, as many as three out of >four family members may have childhood. > >Impressive evidence of a genetic component of childhood comes from a >large-scale twin study by Brady and Partridge (1972). These authors studied >over 106 pairs of twins, looking at concordance rates for childhood. Among >identical or monozygotic twins, concordance was unusually high (0.92), i.e., >when one twin was diagnosed with childhood, the other twin was almost always >a child as well. > >Psychological Models > >A considerable number of psychologically-based theories of the development of >childhood exist. They are too numerous to review here. Among the more >familiar models are Seligman's "learned childishness" model. According to >this model, individuals who are treated like children eventually give up and >become children. As a counterpoint to such theories, some experts have >claimed that childhood does not really exist. Szasz (1980) has called >"childhood" an expedient label. In seeking conformity, we handicap those whom >we find unruly or too short to deal with by labelling them "children." > >Treatment of Childhood: > >Efforts to treat childhood are as old as the syndrome itself. Only in modern >times, however, have humane and systematic treatment protocols been applied. >In part, this increased attention to the problem may be due to the sheer >number of individuals suffering from childhood. Government statistics (DHHS) >reveal that there are more children alive today than at any time in our >history. To paraphrase P.T. Barnum: "There's a child born every minute." > >The overwhelming number of children has made government intervention >inevitable. The nineteenth century saw the institution of what remains the >largest single program for the treatment of childhood -- so-called "public >schools." Under this colossal program, individuals are placed into treatment >groups based on the severity of their condition. For example, those most >severely afflicted may be placed in a "kindergarten" program. Patients at >this level are typically short, unruly, emotionally immature,and >intellectually deficient. Given this type of individual, therapy is >essentially one of patient management and of helping the child master basic >skills (e.g. finger-painting). > >Unfortunately, the "school" system has been largely ineffective. Not only is >the program a massive tax burden, but it has failed even to slow down the >rising incidence of childhood. > >Faced with this failure and the growing epidemic of childhood, mental health >professionals are devoting increasing attention to the treatment of >childhood. Given a theoretical framework by Freud's landmark treatises on >childhood, child psychiatrists and psychologists claimed great successes in >their clinical interventions. > >By the 1950's, however, the clinicians' optimism had waned. Even after years >of costly analysis, many victims remained children. The following case (taken >from Gumbie & Poke, 1957) is typical. > > Billy J., age 8, was brought to treatment by his parents. > Billy's affliction was painfully obvious. He stood only > 4'3" high and weighed a scant 70 lbs., despite the fact > that he ate voraciously. Billy presented a variety of > troubling symptoms. His voice was noticeably high for a > man. He displayed legume anorexia, and, according to his > parents, often refused to bathe. His intellectual > functioning was also below normal -- he had little general > knowledge and could barely write a structured sentence. > Social skills were also deficient. He often spoke > inappropriately and exhibited "whining behaviour." His > sexual experience was non-existent. Indeed, Billy > considered women "icky." His parents reported that his > condition had been present from birth, improving gradually > after he was placed in a school at age 5. The diagnosis > was "primary childhood." After years of painstaking > treatment, Billy improved gradually. At age 11, his height > and weight have increased, his social skills are broader, > and he is now functional enough to hold down a "paper > route." > >After years of this kind of frustration, startling new evidence has come to >light which suggests that the prognosis in cases of childhood may not be all >gloom. A critical review by Fudd (1972) noted that studies of the childhood >syndrome tend to lack careful follow-up. Acting on this observation, Moe, >Larrie, and Kirly (1974) began a large-scale longitudinal study. These >investigators studied two groups. The first group consisted of 34 children >currently engaged in a long-term conventional treatment program. The second >was a group of 42 children receiving no treatment. All subjects had been >diagnosed as children at least 4 years previously, with a mean duration of >childhood of 6.4 years. > >At the end of one year, the results confirmed the clinical wisdom that >childhood is a refractory disorder -- virtually all symptoms persisted and >the treatment group was only slightly better off than the controls. > >The results, however, of a careful 10-year follow-up were startling. The >investigators (Moe, Larrie, Kirly , & Shemp, 1984) assessed the original >cohort on a variety of measures. General knowledge and emotional maturity >were assessed with standard measures. Height was assessed by the "metric >system" (see Ruler, 1923), and legume appetite by the Vegetable Appetite Test >(VAT) designed by Popeye (1968). Moe et al. found that subjects improved >uniformly on all measures. Indeed, in most cases, the subjects appeared to be >symptom-free. Moe et al. report a spontaneous remission rate of 95%, a >finding which is certain to revolutionize the clinical approach to childhood. > >These recent results suggests that the prognosis for victims of childhood may >not be so bad as we have feared. We must not, however, become too complacent. >Despite its apparently high spontaneous remission rate, childhood remains one >of the most serious and rapidly growing disorders facing mental health >professional today. And, beyond the psychological pain it brings, childhood >has recently been linked to a number of physical disorders. Twenty years ago, >Howdi, Doodi, and Beauzeau (1965) demonstrated a six-fold increased risk of >chicken pox, measles, and mumps among children as compared with normal >controls. Later, Barby and Kenn (1971) linked childhood to an elevated risk >of accidents -- compared with normal adults, victims of childhood were much >more likely to scrape their knees, lose their teeth, and fall off their >bikes. Clearly, much more research is needed before we can give any real hope >to the millions of victims wracked by this insidious disorder. > >REFERENCES > >* American Psychiatric Association (1990). The diagnostic and statistical >manual of mental disorders, 4th edition: A preliminary report. Washington, >D.C.; APA. >* Barby, B., & Kenn, K. (1971). The plasticity of behaviour. In B. >* Barby & K. Kenn (Eds.), Psychotherapies R Us. Detroit: Ronco press. >* Brady, C., & Partridge, S. (1972). My dads bigger than your dad. Acta Eur. >Age, 9, 123-126. >* Flintstone, F., & Jetson, G. (1939). Cognitive mediation of labour >disputes. Industrial Psychology Today, 2, 23-35. >* Fudd, E.J. (1972). Locus of control and shoe-size. Journal of Footwear >Psychology, 78, 345-356. >* Gumbie, G., & Pokey, P. (1957). A cognitive theory of iron-smelting. >Journal of Abnormal Metallurgy, 45, 235-239. >* Howdi, C., Doodi, C., & Beauzeau, C. (1965). Western civilization: A >review of the literature. Reader's digest, 60, 23-25. >* Moe, R., Larrie, T., & Kirly, Q. (1974). State childhood vs. trait >childhood. TV guide, May 12-19, 1-3. >* Moe, R., Larrie, T., Kirly, Q., & Shemp, C. (1984). Spontaneous remission >of childhood In W.C. Fields (Ed.), New hope for children and animals. >Hollywood: Acme Press. >* Popeye, T.S.M. (1957). The use of spinach in extreme circumstances. >Journal of Vegetable Science, 58, 530-538. >* Popeye, T.S.M. (1968). Spinach: A phenomenological perspective. >Existential botany, 35, 908-813. >* Rogers, F. (1979). Becoming my neighbour. New York:Soft press. >* Ruler, Y. (1923). Assessing measurements protocols by the multi-method >multiple regression index for the psychometric analysis of factorial >interaction. Annals of Boredom, 67, 1190-1260. >* Spanky, D., & Alfalfa, Q. (1978). Coping with puberty. Sears catalogue, >45-46. >* Suess, D.R. (1983). A psychometric analysis of green eggs with and without >ham. Journal of clinical cuisine, 245, 567-578. >* Temple-Black, S. (1982). Childhood: an ever-so sad disorder. Journal of >precocity, 3, 129-134. >* Tom, C., & Jerry, M. (1967). Human behaviour as a model for understanding >the rat. In M. de Sade (Ed.). The rewards of Punishment. Paris:Bench press. > >FURTHER READINGS > >* Christ, J.H. (1980). Grandiosity in children. Journal of applied theology, >1, 1-1000. >* Joe, G.I. (1965). Aggressive fantasy as wish fulfilment. Archives of >General MacArthur, 5, 23-45. >* Leary, T. (1969). Pharmacotherapy for childhood. Annals of astrological >Science, 67, 456-459. >* Kissoff, K.G.B. (1975). Extinction of learnt behaviour. Paper presented to >the Siberian Psychological Association, 38th annual Annual meeting, >Kamchatka. >* Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1979). Behaviour therapy prevents tooth decay. >Journal of behavioral Orthodontics, 5, 79-89. >* Potash, S., & Hoser, B. (1980). A failure to replicate the results of >Smythe and Barnes. Journal of dental psychiatry, 34, 678-680. >* Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1980). Your study was poorly done: A reply to >Potash and Hoser. Annual review of Aquatic psychiatry, 10, 123-156. >* Potash, S., & Hoser, B. (1981). Your mother wears army boots: A further >reply to Smythe and Barnes. Archives of invective research, 56, 5-9. >* Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1982). Embarrassing moments in the sex lives of >Potash and Hoser: A further reply. National Enquirer, May 16. >